Stereotyping

opertv - public domainA stereotype is defined as a social perception of an individual in terms of group membership or physical attributes. It is a generalization that is made about a group and then attributed to members of that group. Such a generalization may be either positive or negative. For example, women are talented speakers or women are bad drivers. Stereotyping is a form of social categorization that affects the behaviour of those who hold the stereotype, and those who are labeled by a stereotype. Researchers explain stereotyping as a result of schema processing.

Apply your knowledge

Find two examples of stereotypes in the media—newspapers, magazines, books, packaging, products, posters, or films. Bring the image to class, and explain why the image represents a stereotype and why you think this image persists.

The formation of stereotypes

How is it that stereotypes develop? Although Tajfel argues that this is a natural cognitive process of social categorization, this does not explain how the stereotype is actually formed. Schneider (2004) argues that there are two ways that stereotypes develop: indirectly, as a product of our culture or society or directly, as a result of own experience with other people.  Campbell (1967) maintains that there are two key sources of stereotypes: personal experience with individuals and groups, and gatekeepers - the media, parents, and other members of our culture. He goes on to argue that stereotypes thus have a basis in some reality. His grain of truth hypothesis argues that an experience with an individual from a group will then be generalized to the group.

Hamilton and Gifford (1976) argue, instead, that stereotypes are the result of an illusory correlation - that is, people see a relationship between two variables even when there is none. An example of this is when people form false associations between membership of a social group and specific behaviours such as women’s inferior ability in mathematics. The illusory correlation phenomenon causes people to overestimate a link between the two variables, here “women” and “ability in mathematics”. Illusory correlations can come in many forms and culturally-based prejudice about social groups can to some extent be classified as illusory correlations. Illusory correlation is an example of what researchers call “cognitive bias”, that is, a person’s tendency to make errors in judgment based on cognitive factors. Attribution errors are also examples of cognitive bias.

Research in psychology: Hamilton & Gifford (1976)

Hamilton & Gifford (1976) carried out an experiment where participants listened to a series of statements made about people from two groups - simply called group A and B. There were twice as many people in group A (26) as group B (13), so group B was the minority group. Each statement was about one individual in one of the two groups; the statement was either positive or negative. Each group had the same proportion of positive and negative comments.

Participants were then asked how many of the people in each group had positive vs. negative traits. They overestimated the number of negative traits in the minority group. Hamilton & Gifford argued that this was because the minority group was by nature smaller in number, their negative behaviours appeared more distinct and appear to be representative of the group. So, one minority male is caught stealing and it appears to be related to the fact that he is a minority.  This demonstrates why negative stereotypes may be more common for minority groups than for the majority.   Such research has led to the modern practice in many countries not to report the race or ethnicity of people who have been charged with a crime.

Once illusory correlations are made, people tend to seek out or remember information that supports this relationship. This is an example of confirmation bias. Generally, this means that people tend to overlook information that contradicts what they already believe. In a social context, they pay attention to behaviours that confirm what they believe about a group and ignore those behaviours contrary to their beliefs. Confirmation bias makes stereotypical thinking resistant to change. Snyder and Swann (1978) conducted a study in which they told female college students that they would meet a person who was either introverted (reserved, cool) or extroverted (outgoing, warm). The participants were then asked to prepare a set of questions for the person they were going to meet. In general, participants came up with questions that confirmed their perceptions of introverts and extroverts. Those who thought they were going to meet an introvert asked, “What do you dislike about parties?” or “Are there times you wish you could be more outgoing?” and extroverts were asked, “What do you do to liven up a party?” The researchers concluded that the questions asked confirmed participants’ stereotypes of each personality type so that it became a self-fulfilling prophecy - for example, because they believed he was an introvert they asked him questions which made him appear to be one.

Stereotypes can also be formed as a means of taking on the in-group’s social representation of the out-group. In other words, individuals may conform to the group norms with regard to the “other”. Rogers and Frantz (1962) found that white immigrants to Rhodesia (today’s Zimbabwe) developed more stereotypes and prejudice against the local people the longer they stayed there. They argue that this is because they adopted the social norms that were dominant in the group they were joining in order to fit in. The study, however, was cross-sectional.  In other words, the behaviour of the participants was not measured over time, but instead a "snapshot in time" was taken and the data was compared. We cannot know if there was a significant change in the stereotyping and prejudice of the participants over time as their original attitudes toward the local people were never measured.

Effects of stereotyping

In the cognitive approach we learned that schema have an effect on behaviour.  As stereotyping is a form of schema processing, it should be no surprise to you that stereotyping can affect behaviour. Two ways in which stereotyping may affect behaviour are stereotype threat and memory distortion.

Stereotype threat occurs when one is in a situation where there is a threat of being judged or treated stereotypically, or a fear of doing something that would inadvertently confirm that stereotype. Steele and Aronson (1995) carried out an experiment to see the effect of stereotype threat on performance. They gave a 30-minute verbal test, made up of very difficult multiple-choice questions. When one group was told it was a genuine test of their verbal abilities, African American participants scored significantly lower than European American participants. In another group which was presented with the same test but told that it was not diagnostic of their ability, African American students scored higher than the first group, and their performance was not significantly different from the European American students. However, it was not possible to argue that awareness of racial stereotyping was the actual cause of the difference, so they did one more version of the experiment where the participants had to fill in a personal information questionnaire.  Half of the questionnaires asked the participants to identity their race. African Americans who had to identify their race did poorly; those that did not, did just as well as their European American peers.

Steele & Aronson argue that you don’t need to believe in a stereotype for it to affect your behaviour. Stereotype threat turns on spotlight anxiety, which causes emotional distress and pressure that may undermine performance. Students under the stereotype threat often underperform and this can naturally limit their educational prospects. Spencer et al (1999) tested the effect of the stereotype threat on intellectual performance. The researchers gave a difficult mathematics test to students who were strong in mathematics. They predicted that women under the stereotype threat would underperform compared to the men taking the test. The stereotype threat that women experience in mathematics-performance settings originates from a negative stereotype about women’s mathematics ability, which is quite common in society. For women who are good at mathematics and see mathematics as an important part of their self-definition, such a stereotype threat may result in an interfering pressure in test situations. Spencer et al. found that this was true: women in the experiment significantly underperformed compared with equally qualified men on the difficult mathematics tests. However, when the researchers tested literature skills, the two groups performed equally well. This was because women are not stereotype threatened in this area.

ATL: Be empathetic

Many stereotypes about groups are negative – but some can be surprisingly positive.  For example: Asians are very intelligent, Jews are very good with money or gay men enjoy good food, the arts and travel.

At first glance, these stereotypes appear to be “harmless.”  But look at them again. What are the potential negative effects of such stereotypes on these groups?


Another effect of stereotyping is memory distortion. In a study carried out by Martin & Halverson (1983), the researchers wanted to see if gender stereotyping would influence recall in 5 and 6-year-old children. Each child was shown 16 pictures, half of which depicted a child performing gender-consistent activities (for example, a boy playing with a truck) and half showing children displaying gender-inconsistent behaviours (for example, a girl chopping wood). One week later, they tested the recall of the children to see how many of the photos that they could recall accurately.

The results showed that children easily recalled the sex of the actor for scenes in which actors had performed gender-consistent activities. But when the actor’s behaviour was gender inconsistent, the children often distorted the scene by saying that the actor’s sex was consistent with the activity they recalled - that is, they would remember that it was the boy playing with a truck, when in fact they had been shown a photo of a girl playing with a truck.

In a classic experiment done in 1947, Allport & Postman  wanted to see the effect of stereotyping on recall.  To carry out the experiment, the researchers showed a participant a drawing of a white man holding a razor and threatening a black man on the subway.  The participant was then asked to describe what he had seen to another participant – a process called serial reproduction, one of the same strategies used by Bartlett in his classic War of the Ghosts study.  The process was repeated for up to seven participants.  The researchers found that in over half of the experiments, at some stage the story was retold so that the black man was threatening the white man. It appears that stereotyping had affected the retelling of the story. The study is very old, so is it still relevant today? Unfortunately, the answer is yes.

In a study by Payne (2001), participants were first given a test of racial prejudice to eliminate strong racial prejudice as a confounding variable.  32 non-black university students were asked to sit at a computer screen.  They would be shown a face - either a white or a black face -  followed by an image.  They had only a second to choose whether the image was either a tool or a gun. The study showed that priming the participants with black faces led to more errors in identifying the image as a weapon rather than a tool. Thus, stereotyping affected perception, which then had implications for memory.  Several studies have shown that the race of an alleged criminal influences memory of a weapon. The effect of stereotypes can be very serious indeed.

Checking for understanding

Which of the following is an example of a stereotype?

 

 

George sees a Swedish tourist at his local restaurant.  He watches as the tourist  argues with the waiter about the bill.  When the tourist realizes that the mistake is his, and not the waiter’s, he does not apologize, but storms out of the restaurant. George thinks that all Swedish tourists are rude. Which theory or concept below explains what just happened?

Through his personal experience, George has made a link between being Swedish and being rude.  However, that link is not really there.  If he were to see another rude Swede in his local restaurant, he may then experience confirmation bias - noting this behaviour and reinforcing his belief that all Swedes are rude.  You will be happy to know that Swedes are not rude and the Swede in this story was probably having a bad day. :)

 

What is a key limitation of Hamilton & Gifford’s (1976) study?

The study has low ecological validity. We don't usually make decisions about a group of people because we are asked to by a researcher after a series of random statements about groups.  Although we may draw conclusions about a group after reading about them on the Internet, watching television or hearing comments from others, the methodology of the experiment is questionable with regard to its ecological validity because it is highly controlled and removes any other potential variables that would affect how we think about a group - eg. personal appearance, historical relationships, etc.

 

What does it mean when we say that Rogers & Frantz’s study was cross-sectional?

The study only looked at data from a single "snap shot" in time.  It was not longitudinal, so it does not demonstrate change over time.  The response about "all types of people" would mean that the study used a "stratified sample."  A study that compares the results of several different studies is known as a meta-analysis.

 

In a study of geography skills, John was asked to read an article about how poorly American score on world geography tests compared to Europeans.   He doesn’t believe that the article is true.  What can we predict will be the results of his geography test?

Steele and Aronson argue that even when we don't believe in a stereotype, it still affects our performance in a negative manner.

 

What was the control used by Payne in his study of stereotypes and memory?

 

 

What research method was used in Martin & Halvorson's study of gender stereotyping and memory?

The children were allocated to groups based on their own gender - they were not randomly allocated.  This is a defining feature of a quasi-experiment.

 

Total Score:

Culture, behaviour and cognition
All materials on this website are for the exclusive use of teachers and students at subscribing schools for the period of their subscription. Any unauthorised copying or posting of materials on other websites is an infringement of our copyright and could result in your account being blocked and legal action being taken against you.

Comments 11

James Meyer 12 March 2018 - 14:36

The description of Hamilton and Gifford's study in the summary presented in the text does not mention slides, but the comprehension question does.

John Crane 13 March 2018 - 05:17

Thanks for catching that, James. I will fix it.

Ian Latham 31 May 2018 - 14:55

For 'explain one theory of stereotypes' I've had the illusory correlation, heuristics, group membership, etc. One student has stated that they 'can be considered a result of schema theory....one study that supports the theory of memory distortion is' M&H and give an accurate description before concluding 'memory distortion as a result of stereotypes is possible' and reiterating why. Does this count as a relevant answer? If not, a 5 or only a 3?

They don't explain what schema theory means or how memory distortion might be evidence of schemas at work. If they had, would that mean a 7?

John Crane 1 June 2018 - 05:02

Dear Ian

The study is a study of "effects of stereotyping," so it is not really appropriate for an "origins" question. Stereotypes as a result of schema is rather shaky ground. Where does it come from? It would be better to talk about social cognitive theory or gender schema theory which discusses how they are actually formed. A stereotype is a type of schema. I would say that this response would probably earn in the low markband.

Ian Latham 1 June 2018 - 08:46

Thanks. Are you saying that the SAQ 'explain one theory of stereotypes' can only be about origin, rather than about origin or effect?

I'd understood 'a theory of stereotyping' to mean any theory related to stereotyping: ie. how it 'works'. Typical answers in my exam that I'd marked as 'on target' are the illusory correlation (origin) or spotlight anxiety leading to impaired performance (effect). Is the 'stereotype effect' answer with Steele & Aronson incorrect?

As far as SCT is concerned, it is also a completely separate topic. If a student briefly explains in an introduction that SCT explains the formation of stereotypes and then goes on to explain Bandura and to link this to stereotypes in general and gender stereotypes in particular (given participant's comments about what is usual for a woman or not), what maximum mark would they get?

John Crane 2 June 2018 - 08:37

Dear Ian,

Sorry, I misread your explanation above. I guess I need to stop replying at 5:00 am! So, yes, effects or origins are appropriate for the question. So, M & H would be fine in an "effects of stereotyping" approach, as long as this is how it was framed as a response. Depending on how well the study is described, it could earn up to top marks. But it sounds like the student has not really explained the way that schema play a role so my guess is that a 5 is most likely the right score.

The studies used are supposed to be directly linked to the question. If a student used Bandura do discuss the role of gender stereotypes - and that it affected whether a child mimicked the behaviour of the adult or not - then it should earn marks. If it used to demonstrate how stereotypes are formed, then it would not.

Dabalina Chatterjee 2 August 2018 - 16:24

Dear John.
Discuss two theories of stereotyping - can you just guide me with this question

John Crane 3 August 2018 - 05:35

Dear Dabalina,

First, remember to look at the lesson plan here: thinkib.net

For example, students may choose between SIT, illusory correlation and conformity to group norms. They should describe the theory, the study and then evaluate the study. The discussion would then focus on the difficulties of studying stereotypes. For example, the SIT is difficult to observe in action, the theories seem to be more explanatory than predictive - they do not explain why some people do NOT develop stereotypes, the ethics of carrying out research that induces stereotyping....

Dabalina Chatterjee 5 August 2018 - 15:31

okay. thank you . got your point

Sara Dean 29 August 2018 - 13:06

Hi John,

Will the previous studies/concepts related to effects of stereotyping still work here (Memory distortion - Allport & Postman and Martin & Halvorson; Diagnosis - Lewis and Fernando)?

THANK YOU!

John Crane 29 August 2018 - 17:59

Dear Sara - yes, they would.